I specifically wanted to work several themes into my current project:
(1) Clusivity outside of the normal first person plural inclusive/exclusive distinction;
(2) An unusual but natural number system;
(3) A distance contrast expanding beyond third person pronouns/demonstratives;
(4) Differential (quirky) case marking of subjects and objects; and
(5) After reading Aikhenvald, use of non-numeral classifiers.
(1) & (2) Clusivity and Number.
More: show
First, because this will get confusing, I will refer to clusivity as follows: 1-3 = the speaker and possibly third persons; 1-2 = the speaker and at least one addressee; 2-2 = at least two addressees but possibly multiple addresses; 2-3 = at least one addressee and possibly third persons. 3-3 follow the same logic. I will refer to number as follows: minimal, augmented, and, for limited items, anti-dual.
Minimal means the minimum number of any item/group, which typically lines up with Singular. It also includes (essentially dual) 1-2 and 2-3 pronouns; however, within 1-3, the Minimal refers to only the speaker. Further, Minimal includes terms for paired body parts, dyadic kinship terms, and other natural pairs. For these later (non-pronouns), there is a minor number system: the anti-dual, which is used to single out a specific member in a pair. Augmented includes greater amounts: inclusive pronouns with greater than two referents, multiple dyads, pairs, etc. The augmented may be analyzed as occurring in two types: additively augmented and associatively augmented (either by addressees or third persons). The two Augmentatives overlap with some items as displayed below.
The numbers extend as follows:
1-2 ----- 2-2 ----- 1-3 ----- 2-3 ----- 3-3 ----- kin/Names ----- Dyadic Kin ----- Paired nouns ----- common nouns
<-------------------------------------------------minimal----------------------------------------------------------------------->
<2 Assoc Aug>...................................................................................................................
.....................<------------------3 Associatively Augmented------------>.............................................
....................................................<-----------------Additively Augmented--------------------------------->
.......................................................................<------------Anti-Dual------------->......................
For example: */m-ũ:-d:-0/ --> /mũ:d:/ == 1st-2d.sg.nom or 1-2nd.min.nom; */t-ĩ:-lõŋg-0/ --> /tĩ:lõŋg/ == 2nd-3rd.prox.pl.nom or 2-3rd.aug.prox.nom.
Minimal means the minimum number of any item/group, which typically lines up with Singular. It also includes (essentially dual) 1-2 and 2-3 pronouns; however, within 1-3, the Minimal refers to only the speaker. Further, Minimal includes terms for paired body parts, dyadic kinship terms, and other natural pairs. For these later (non-pronouns), there is a minor number system: the anti-dual, which is used to single out a specific member in a pair. Augmented includes greater amounts: inclusive pronouns with greater than two referents, multiple dyads, pairs, etc. The augmented may be analyzed as occurring in two types: additively augmented and associatively augmented (either by addressees or third persons). The two Augmentatives overlap with some items as displayed below.
The numbers extend as follows:
1-2 ----- 2-2 ----- 1-3 ----- 2-3 ----- 3-3 ----- kin/Names ----- Dyadic Kin ----- Paired nouns ----- common nouns
<-------------------------------------------------minimal----------------------------------------------------------------------->
<2 Assoc Aug>...................................................................................................................
.....................<------------------3 Associatively Augmented------------>.............................................
....................................................<-----------------Additively Augmented--------------------------------->
.......................................................................<------------Anti-Dual------------->......................
For example: */m-ũ:-d:-0/ --> /mũ:d:/ == 1st-2d.sg.nom or 1-2nd.min.nom; */t-ĩ:-lõŋg-0/ --> /tĩ:lõŋg/ == 2nd-3rd.prox.pl.nom or 2-3rd.aug.prox.nom.
More: show
Taken from my CC Quickies Post -- 1st.sg is inherently exclusive -- most analyses just don't bother to note that. I think the real question, is why don't we see 1st.sg inclusives that often in analyses.
---
Some languages with the clusivity feature do not structure their pronouns on a singular v. dual v. plural basis. Instead, they use what is called a minimal v. unit augmented v. augmented number system.
In more familiar languages using singular-plural, the following pattern as sets:
without dual:
(1) 1st.sg, 2nd.sg;
(2) 1st.pl.ex, 1st.pl.inc., 2nd.pl.
With dual:
(1) 1st.sg, 2nd.sg, etc;
(2) 1st.du.ex, 1st.du.inc, 2nd.du;
(3) 1st.pl.ex, 1st.pl.inc., 2nd.pl.
Case/number markers in these languages often -- though certainly not always -- are most similar within these sets. Often one or both of the 1st person plurals have a stem totally unrelated to the singulars.
In languages using a minimal-augmented system, the following pattern as sets:
Without unit-aug/dual: (See Ilocano for pretty much this system analyzed as a singular-plural language. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Ilokano_g ... l_pronouns)
(1) 1st.min = 1st.sg.ex, 1st.inc.min = 1st.inc.du, 2nd.min = 2nd.sg;
(2)1st.aug = 1st.pl.ex, 1st.inc.aug = 1st.pl.inc, 2nd.aug = 2nd.pl
With unit-aug/dual:
(1) 1st.ex.min = 1st.sg.ex, 1st.inc.min = 1st.inc.du, 2nd.min = 2nd.sg;
(2) 1st.ex.unit-aug = 1st.du.ex, 1st.inc.unit-aug = 1st.paucal(trial?).inc, 2nd.unit-aug = 2nd.du;
(3) 1st.ex.aug = 1st.pl.ex, 1st.inc.aug = 1st.pl.inc, 2nd.aug = 2nd.pl.
Here again, Case/number markers in this languages often -- though certainly not always -- are most similar within these sets. So, a "trial" inclusive will look like duals elsewhere in the language.
Note how 1st.inc is almost treated like a separate person. Languages with systems like this often show up as having odd number systems in the 1st persons (and sometimes other pronouns). Notice for instance the paucal~trial. For this type of language it seems better to analyze a minimal augmented system rather than to have a "missing" singular and a strange trial number, which exists no-where else in the language.
Tongan's pronouns don't quite fit into the reanalysis. Note that it is instead the 1st.ex pronouns that seem to be bucking the system. The source notes that it appears semantically that the various first singulars are a sort of T-V pair.
---
Some languages with the clusivity feature do not structure their pronouns on a singular v. dual v. plural basis. Instead, they use what is called a minimal v. unit augmented v. augmented number system.
In more familiar languages using singular-plural, the following pattern as sets:
without dual:
(1) 1st.sg, 2nd.sg;
(2) 1st.pl.ex, 1st.pl.inc., 2nd.pl.
With dual:
(1) 1st.sg, 2nd.sg, etc;
(2) 1st.du.ex, 1st.du.inc, 2nd.du;
(3) 1st.pl.ex, 1st.pl.inc., 2nd.pl.
Case/number markers in these languages often -- though certainly not always -- are most similar within these sets. Often one or both of the 1st person plurals have a stem totally unrelated to the singulars.
In languages using a minimal-augmented system, the following pattern as sets:
Without unit-aug/dual: (See Ilocano for pretty much this system analyzed as a singular-plural language. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Ilokano_g ... l_pronouns)
(1) 1st.min = 1st.sg.ex, 1st.inc.min = 1st.inc.du, 2nd.min = 2nd.sg;
(2)1st.aug = 1st.pl.ex, 1st.inc.aug = 1st.pl.inc, 2nd.aug = 2nd.pl
With unit-aug/dual:
(1) 1st.ex.min = 1st.sg.ex, 1st.inc.min = 1st.inc.du, 2nd.min = 2nd.sg;
(2) 1st.ex.unit-aug = 1st.du.ex, 1st.inc.unit-aug = 1st.paucal(trial?).inc, 2nd.unit-aug = 2nd.du;
(3) 1st.ex.aug = 1st.pl.ex, 1st.inc.aug = 1st.pl.inc, 2nd.aug = 2nd.pl.
Here again, Case/number markers in this languages often -- though certainly not always -- are most similar within these sets. So, a "trial" inclusive will look like duals elsewhere in the language.
Note how 1st.inc is almost treated like a separate person. Languages with systems like this often show up as having odd number systems in the 1st persons (and sometimes other pronouns). Notice for instance the paucal~trial. For this type of language it seems better to analyze a minimal augmented system rather than to have a "missing" singular and a strange trial number, which exists no-where else in the language.
Tongan's pronouns don't quite fit into the reanalysis. Note that it is instead the 1st.ex pronouns that seem to be bucking the system. The source notes that it appears semantically that the various first singulars are a sort of T-V pair.
More: show
Essentially the pronoun morphemes on independent and clitic pronouns are highly fused for person and number – as well as deixis for 3d person, which I skip over here.
I do not like to think of the combinations person as seperate persons, but rather combinations of (1) person features: speaker, listener and/or non-speech act participan; and (2) number: minimal or associatively augmented.
So, the person.number series can be arranged as:
1+2 (+2) -- minimal: speaker plus one listener; augmented: speaker associated with more than one listeners.
2+2 (+2) -- minimal: two listeners; augmented: more than two associated listeners.
1 (+3) -- minimal: one speaker; augmented: speaker associated with at least one non-speech act participant.
2 (+3) -- minimal: one listener; augmented: one listener associated with at least one non-speech act participant.
3 (+3) -- minimal: one non-speech act participant; augmented: associated non-speech act participants
Morphologically, the language does not make a 1+2 (+3) distinction or other finer grained variations; however, forms could be easily made with coordination.
Note that minimal and augmented are not the same as singular and non-singular; so, for example, the “first person inclusive” (1+2 below) with two speech act participants is logically a dual but here is a minimal since it patterns with 2nd “singular” (2 below) and 3rd “singular” (3 below). The language also use verbal number marking to note whether a minimum or augmented number of participants are involved; so, for example, |1.min 2.min SAW.min| AND |1st.min 2+3.aug SAW.aug| are valid but |*1st.min 2nd.aug SAW.min| does not.
I do not like to think of the combinations person as seperate persons, but rather combinations of (1) person features: speaker, listener and/or non-speech act participan; and (2) number: minimal or associatively augmented.
So, the person.number series can be arranged as:
1+2 (+2) -- minimal: speaker plus one listener; augmented: speaker associated with more than one listeners.
2+2 (+2) -- minimal: two listeners; augmented: more than two associated listeners.
1 (+3) -- minimal: one speaker; augmented: speaker associated with at least one non-speech act participant.
2 (+3) -- minimal: one listener; augmented: one listener associated with at least one non-speech act participant.
3 (+3) -- minimal: one non-speech act participant; augmented: associated non-speech act participants
Morphologically, the language does not make a 1+2 (+3) distinction or other finer grained variations; however, forms could be easily made with coordination.
Note that minimal and augmented are not the same as singular and non-singular; so, for example, the “first person inclusive” (1+2 below) with two speech act participants is logically a dual but here is a minimal since it patterns with 2nd “singular” (2 below) and 3rd “singular” (3 below). The language also use verbal number marking to note whether a minimum or augmented number of participants are involved; so, for example, |1.min 2.min SAW.min| AND |1st.min 2+3.aug SAW.aug| are valid but |*1st.min 2nd.aug SAW.min| does not.
More: show
In most languages (all natlangs?), spatial deixis is restricted to the 3rd person. In TEST, spatial deixis is also found in 1-3 and 2-3 Associatively Augmented pronouns for diachronic reasons -- essentially the plural form in these persons is derived from a fused form of "these.people" and "those.people" respectively.
1-2 ----- 2-2 ----- 1-3 ----- 2-3 ----- 3-3 ----- kin/Names ----- Dyadic Kin ----- Paired nouns ----- common nouns
.....................<-----------------Plural Spatial Deixis ------------------->................................................
..............................<-Sing. Deixis->....................................................................................
Again, for example: */t-ĩ:-lõŋg-0/ --> /tĩ:lõŋg/ == 2nd-3rd.prox.pl.nom or 2-3rd.aug.prox.nom.
1-2 ----- 2-2 ----- 1-3 ----- 2-3 ----- 3-3 ----- kin/Names ----- Dyadic Kin ----- Paired nouns ----- common nouns
.....................<-----------------Plural Spatial Deixis ------------------->................................................
..............................<-Sing. Deixis->....................................................................................
Again, for example: */t-ĩ:-lõŋg-0/ --> /tĩ:lõŋg/ == 2nd-3rd.prox.pl.nom or 2-3rd.aug.prox.nom.
More: show
TEST, as with most of my projects, has a tangled morphosyntactic alignment. In addition, Actors and Subjects are frequently marked with non-core cases or as PPs.
Subjects can be marked according to two variables: (1) experiencer v. non-experiencer (i.e. typical agents), and (2) volitional v. non-volitional. The occurrence of Experiencer A/S is largely selected by the verb but may occur semantically with others. Volition is largely semantically selected; however, this pattern of marking may also overlap with certain antecedents.
(1) Volitional Non-experiencers (Agents) are not marked and occur in nominate or ergative case.
(2) Volitional Experiencers are not marked and occur as dative PPs.
(3) Non-volitional Non-experiencers are marked and occur as instrumental PPs.
(4) Non-volitional experiencers are marked and occur in locative case or as locative PPs.
Subjects can be marked according to two variables: (1) experiencer v. non-experiencer (i.e. typical agents), and (2) volitional v. non-volitional. The occurrence of Experiencer A/S is largely selected by the verb but may occur semantically with others. Volition is largely semantically selected; however, this pattern of marking may also overlap with certain antecedents.
(1) Volitional Non-experiencers (Agents) are not marked and occur in nominate or ergative case.
(2) Volitional Experiencers are not marked and occur as dative PPs.
(3) Non-volitional Non-experiencers are marked and occur as instrumental PPs.
(4) Non-volitional experiencers are marked and occur in locative case or as locative PPs.
More: show
A large number of languages have numeral classifiers (a/k/a counter words). Further possibilities exist, such as the classificatory verbs in certain Athabascan languages (a/k/a handling verbs), which classify the object of a verb in terms of shape, size, etc. Also of note are the possessive classifiers of certain Oceanic languages such as Pohnpeian.
Classification in TEST falls into two systems. Within these respective systems, classifiers forms largely overlap and occur in differing roles.
System I: (Possessive Classifiers (of the Possessed and Relational Subtypes) and Comitative Classifiers)
The possessive classifiers fuse with the genitive preposition and classify humans, particularly relatives, clothing and personal adornments, and body parts. Comitative classifiers fuse with the comitative preposition and classify humans, again particularly relatives.
System II: (Verbal Classifiers, Locative Classifiers, and Demonstrative Classifiers)
The verbal classifiers fuse with the verb (or certain participles in AUX-constructions) and classify body parts on one hand or location and relative motion and position on the other. Locative classifiers cover the same field but fuse with locative postpositions (never with the locative case). Finally Demonstrative classifiers fuse with demonstratives (and certain pronouns) and classify location and relative motion.
Classification in TEST falls into two systems. Within these respective systems, classifiers forms largely overlap and occur in differing roles.
System I: (Possessive Classifiers (of the Possessed and Relational Subtypes) and Comitative Classifiers)
The possessive classifiers fuse with the genitive preposition and classify humans, particularly relatives, clothing and personal adornments, and body parts. Comitative classifiers fuse with the comitative preposition and classify humans, again particularly relatives.
System II: (Verbal Classifiers, Locative Classifiers, and Demonstrative Classifiers)
The verbal classifiers fuse with the verb (or certain participles in AUX-constructions) and classify body parts on one hand or location and relative motion and position on the other. Locative classifiers cover the same field but fuse with locative postpositions (never with the locative case). Finally Demonstrative classifiers fuse with demonstratives (and certain pronouns) and classify location and relative motion.
More: show
I’m a bit confused on what you are looking for. Gender is, perhaps, not the word you are looking for. Can you give some more examples please?
“Classification” marked on verbs typically refers to classificatory verbs (some Na-Dene languages), verbal classifier affixes (widespread), classificatory noun incorporation, or noun class agreement on verbs (see Caucasian verb prefixes or nonfinite verbs in many languages). All of these strategies agree with arguments rather than assign marking to those arguments as you are suggesting.
So, for example, verbal classifier affixes might appear as:
3d.INANIM COPULA-stick-like -- “it’s a stick”
3d.ANIM 3d.INANIM break-IMPFV COPULA-horizontal.plane “he is breaking a horizontal object” or perhaps, “he is plowing”
Or classificatory incorporation example might appear as:
3d.ANIM stick-break-IMPFV COPULA “he is stick-breaking” or “he is breaking sticks”
Similarly – but moving from sticks to humans – you might have:
3d.ANIM woman-see-IMPFV COPULA “he is woman-seeing” or “he sees a woman,” etc.
The type of cla
Here you have something very different. Instead, it seems you have a system where the marked v. unmarked constructions are based on whether the gender of the argument(s) is expected or not. So, as in your first example, “the man sang” is marked (or as you put it: “”man” takes a feminine form”).
Rant one. This reminds me a bit of the marking of the arguments of transitive verbs direct-inverse systems. In D-I systems, variations between marked and unmarked constructions are triggered by variations in the expected animacy or discourse salience of third and sometimes second person constituents. So, for example looking at an animacy based system, “the human kicked the dog” is not marked, but “the dog kicked the human” is marked; or, looking at a discourse based system, “the human (who is the current focus of discourse) kicked the other human” is unmarked, but “the human (who has been background) kicked the other human (who is the current focus of discourse)” is marked.
Rant two. Another thing this reminds me a bit of is noun classifiers which are not to be confused with gender or noun classes. Noun classifiers typically have a discoursed based role. Generally, a noun classifier is related to the classified noun in a generic-specific pair; so, for example, you might have “respected.human uncle” or, using multiple noun classifiers, “small animal.object threat.to.life snake.” Depending on the language, once introduced into discourse, either the classified noun or the classifier may be left off. Languages vary on the use of noun classifiers a good bit. So in a following text you might get :
Sentence 1: “respected.human uncle small animal.object threat.to.life snake see.PFV – uncle saw a snake.
Sentence 2 (meta-language A, which drops the classified noun while it remains topical): “respected.human small animal.object kill.IMPF. – “he is killing it” ~ "the respected one is killing the little thing"
Sentence 2 (meta-language B, which drops the classifier once the noun is introduced into the discourse): “uncle snake kill.IMPV – uncle (introduced into discourse) is killing the snake (introduced into discourse).
For what you are describing, I could see a system where a noun classifier is required if the noun class of the classified noun is topical – i.e. not the noun class typically associated with the verb. So, for example:
Man-Uncle song sing.IMPF -- "uncle is singing song(s)!!!! (gasp)"
Man-Uncle robbers fight.IMPF -- "uncle is fighting robbers"
But
Woman-aunt song sing.IMPF -- "aunt is singing song(s)"
Woman-aunt robbers fight.IMPF -- "aunt is fighting robbers!!!!"
Or, alternatively,
Sentence 1 "woman-Aunt arrived"
Sentence "sang" -- She sang (here, I assume the meta-language is pro-drop and, since there is no classifier, the A/S must be female so the refernce would fall back to Aunt from the prior sentence)
Sentence 1" "man-Uncle arrived"
Sentence 2: "man(-Uncle) sings" (here, the male classifier must be used since uncle is not the expected A/S of the verb.)
In general, I’d recommend reading “Classifiers” which covers gender/noun classes and much else.
“Classification” marked on verbs typically refers to classificatory verbs (some Na-Dene languages), verbal classifier affixes (widespread), classificatory noun incorporation, or noun class agreement on verbs (see Caucasian verb prefixes or nonfinite verbs in many languages). All of these strategies agree with arguments rather than assign marking to those arguments as you are suggesting.
So, for example, verbal classifier affixes might appear as:
3d.INANIM COPULA-stick-like -- “it’s a stick”
3d.ANIM 3d.INANIM break-IMPFV COPULA-horizontal.plane “he is breaking a horizontal object” or perhaps, “he is plowing”
Or classificatory incorporation example might appear as:
3d.ANIM stick-break-IMPFV COPULA “he is stick-breaking” or “he is breaking sticks”
Similarly – but moving from sticks to humans – you might have:
3d.ANIM woman-see-IMPFV COPULA “he is woman-seeing” or “he sees a woman,” etc.
The type of cla
Here you have something very different. Instead, it seems you have a system where the marked v. unmarked constructions are based on whether the gender of the argument(s) is expected or not. So, as in your first example, “the man sang” is marked (or as you put it: “”man” takes a feminine form”).
Rant one. This reminds me a bit of the marking of the arguments of transitive verbs direct-inverse systems. In D-I systems, variations between marked and unmarked constructions are triggered by variations in the expected animacy or discourse salience of third and sometimes second person constituents. So, for example looking at an animacy based system, “the human kicked the dog” is not marked, but “the dog kicked the human” is marked; or, looking at a discourse based system, “the human (who is the current focus of discourse) kicked the other human” is unmarked, but “the human (who has been background) kicked the other human (who is the current focus of discourse)” is marked.
Rant two. Another thing this reminds me a bit of is noun classifiers which are not to be confused with gender or noun classes. Noun classifiers typically have a discoursed based role. Generally, a noun classifier is related to the classified noun in a generic-specific pair; so, for example, you might have “respected.human uncle” or, using multiple noun classifiers, “small animal.object threat.to.life snake.” Depending on the language, once introduced into discourse, either the classified noun or the classifier may be left off. Languages vary on the use of noun classifiers a good bit. So in a following text you might get :
Sentence 1: “respected.human uncle small animal.object threat.to.life snake see.PFV – uncle saw a snake.
Sentence 2 (meta-language A, which drops the classified noun while it remains topical): “respected.human small animal.object kill.IMPF. – “he is killing it” ~ "the respected one is killing the little thing"
Sentence 2 (meta-language B, which drops the classifier once the noun is introduced into the discourse): “uncle snake kill.IMPV – uncle (introduced into discourse) is killing the snake (introduced into discourse).
For what you are describing, I could see a system where a noun classifier is required if the noun class of the classified noun is topical – i.e. not the noun class typically associated with the verb. So, for example:
Man-Uncle song sing.IMPF -- "uncle is singing song(s)!!!! (gasp)"
Man-Uncle robbers fight.IMPF -- "uncle is fighting robbers"
But
Woman-aunt song sing.IMPF -- "aunt is singing song(s)"
Woman-aunt robbers fight.IMPF -- "aunt is fighting robbers!!!!"
Or, alternatively,
Sentence 1 "woman-Aunt arrived"
Sentence "sang" -- She sang (here, I assume the meta-language is pro-drop and, since there is no classifier, the A/S must be female so the refernce would fall back to Aunt from the prior sentence)
Sentence 1" "man-Uncle arrived"
Sentence 2: "man(-Uncle) sings" (here, the male classifier must be used since uncle is not the expected A/S of the verb.)
In general, I’d recommend reading “Classifiers” which covers gender/noun classes and much else.